Friday, May 24, 2013

Teachers of the Deaf in Early Intervention

Recommendations for Successful Home Visits by Teachers of the Deaf 



In general, I believe it’s best to have a similar role to what we call a secondary service provider, assuming the family has a general early interventionist (sometimes called a developmental specialist or therapist). It could also be possible, theoretically, for you to be the primary service provider, but we wouldn’t want two people trying to function as primaries. That takes away many of the advantages of a primary-service-provider approach. As secondary, you would make joint home visits with the primary (usually a general early interventionist). 

The focus of those meetings would be on your consultation with the family. The primary is there to ask additional questions, make sure the family understands your ideas, prompt the family to assess the feasibility of your suggestions and to ask for alternatives if necessary, and pay attention to your suggestions, because he or she will be supporting the family in carrying them out. 

As you know only too well, a second and hugely important role is to encourage the family to become proficient in the communication mode they’ve chosen. Ask them to think about what it would be like for a hearing infant or toddler to have parents who could say only more, sit, and cookie. If the early interventionists is any good, he or she knows a number of signs. But nowadays parents can learn to sign in a variety of ways. I would expect that, in addition to teaching them some signs, you also counsel them on ways they can learn without you (e.g., Internet sites, community college classes). Also, you can tell them about the order of signs very young children learn, which is different from the first spoken words hearing children learn. So your expertise is vital, but not weekly—if there’s a general early interventionist—and not just as a signing teacher.

Photo from Special Education Department, Illinois State University

Monday, May 20, 2013

Comments Deleted

Unfortunately, I have had to delete all comments to my posts, because most of them were spam. My apologies to those of you who wanted to make genuine comments. Write to me, and I'll incorporate your comments into future posts.

Attention and Engagement

How are attention and engagement related to each other?

Attention is a component of engagement, so figuring out the relationship between the two is tricky. Some early studies (Dunst, McWilliam, & Holbert, 1986) separated attentional from active engagement but didn’t really look at attention as a stand-alone construct. In the early days of figuring out engagement, I read quite a bit of Michael Lewis’s work.

Within engagement, my colleagues and I pay attention to casual attention, as an indicator of indifferentiated engagement, and focused attention. The former appears to have less value—at least, we put it lower on the developmental hierarchy than we do focused attention.

Can a child spend much time attending yet not be engaged? If it were casual attention (looking around), we’d consider it low engagement. But if it were focused attention, we’d consider it good engagement. Because we work with young children, however, we know that too much focused attention without accompanying manipulative (i.e., active) engagement could be a bad thing. Children with cerebral palsy have shown us that attentional engagement can go only so far; it’s better for a child to have actual control over his or her environment. Attention even more than engagement, therefore, seems necessary but not sufficient. Can a child spend much time engaged yet not be attending? At first, we might think so: The child who is very appropriately active appears not to spend much time just looking. But we do use eye gaze (i.e., looking) to help judge what a child is interested in. So, even in active engagement, attention plays a role.

People who like to categorize children (and don’t we all, in some ways?) might think of some children as watchers and others as doers. We know better than to consider these “learning styles,” yet they might describe how children make sense of their environment. Some might attend first and then experiment; others might jump in first—and then probably never spend a lot of time attending.

Another place where attention has an interesting role is in mastery pleasure, when a child looks at what he or she has accomplished, indicating to us adults that the child finds the accomplishment reinforcing. Compare this to the child who does something and shows no affective response. We don’t know whether that kid found the activity reinforcing or not, unless he or she returns to it, which after all is the definition of reinforcement.

Attention and engagement, therefore, are intertwined but it research is needed to find out more about the connection between the two.